Natural Hazards Center

Quick Response Research Report #246

A Pilot Assessment of Psychological Factors Associated with Hurricane Sandy Preparedness in Post-Earthquake Haiti

Leah E. James, Ph.D.
Research Associate, Natural Hazards Center,
Institute of Behavioral Science, University of Colorado, Boulder


The views expressed in the report are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the Natural Hazards Center or the University of Colorado. Quick Response Research Reports capture perishable data on recent events. All analysis is preliminary.


Suggested Citation: James, Leah E. A pilot assessment of psychological factors associated with Hurricane Sandy preparedness in post-earthquake Haiti. Quick Response Grant Report series; 246. 2013.


Abstract

The current study examined psychological factors associated with engagement in disaster preparedness behaviors among displaced Haitians affected by Hurricane Sandy. Seventy-four semi-structured interviews were conducted in a camp for internally displaced survivors of the 2010 earthquake in metropolitan Port-au-Prince. Multiple regression analyses revealed that prior trauma (including disaster) exposure was negatively associated with preparedness. Avoidance symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) were also negatively associated with preparedness, while other symptoms of PTSD and depression symptoms were not significantly related to preparedness. Trust of sources of preparedness information, disaster preparedness efficacy (perceived possession of necessary finances, time, and knowledge) and perceived financial security overall were also significant predictors. Results suggest that avoidance of traumatic stimuli, including those associated with prior disasters, may impede engagement in preparedness for future disasters and thus imply the need for integration of mental health intervention into disaster preparedness training for chronically disaster-exposed populations.



Introduction and Research Objectives

In late October 2012, Hurricane Sandy swept through Haiti, resulting in floods and landslides that killed more than 50 people, destroyed 200,000 homes, and displaced 32,000 people (IDMC, 2012; USAID 2012). This event was particularly devastating for the approximately 360,000 people already living in camps for internally displaced peoples (IDPs) as a result of the January 2010 earthquake and subsequent disasters. Nearly a fifth of Port-au-Prince’s 541 earthquake IDP camps experienced flooding and shelter damage, affecting 30,000 IDPs (IDMC, 2012; USAID, 2012). Hurricane Sandy was preceded by succession of damaging natural disasters in Haiti, including Hurricane Isaac just three months prior and an earthquake in early 2012. Such disasters have occurred against a backdrop of chronic hardship, including a cholera epidemic that has killed 7500 people since 2010, insecure camp conditions and exposure to physical and sexual violence, inadequate shelter and toilets, food-insecurity, unemployment, and political and social instability (IOM, 2012; 2013).

In light of the high frequency and extreme impact of disasters in Haiti, the implementation of preparedness measures (e.g., basic first aid, community risk assessment, evacuation route mapping and planning for connecting with family members, health and hygiene practices in emergencies, and the use early warning systems) is critical, especially for displaced individuals residing in insecure shelters. The International Organization for Migration (IOM) identifies “lack of knowledge about natural disaster risk management among the population” as one of the primary reasons for Haiti’s vulnerability to severe disaster impact (IOM, 2012, p. 42). The international community has made attempts to bolster both knowledge and resources in Haitian populations (e.g., Red Cross/IOM training of disaster management committees (IOM, 2012) but the extent to which preparedness information is retained by camp residents or adopted in IDP camps is unclear. One of the main challenges facing preparedness activities is building an evidence base which demonstrates success in regards to beneficiary well-being and behavior change. Much of the disaster preparedness literature is based on opinions and anecdotal evidence, rather than empirical evaluation studies (Yeager et al., 2010). This gap is particularly meaningful in light of indications that people don’t always engage in disaster preparedness or mitigation activities, even when they possess sufficient resources, receive preparedness training, and/or have a history of disaster exposure (McKay, 2012). For example, even after the establishment of effective evacuation procedures for IDPs, many Haitian IDPs choose not to evacuate in advance of storms and flooding – despite having experienced devastating disaster impact in the recent past (personal correspondence, IOM-Haiti DRR).

The literature is ambiguous regarding the role of prior disaster exposure in predicting disaster preparedness engagement. In their review, Kellens and colleagues (2013) report that most studies find a positive relationship between hazards exposure and disaster mitigation. However, conflicting findings suggest that a prior history of disaster exposure may actually make a person less likely to prepare (e.g., Lin, Shaw, & Ho, 2008), and that reasons for this are sometimes psychological in nature and dependent on social, cultural, and religious context (Morrissey & Reser, 2003; Reale, 2010). In light of the international community’s enormous financial commitment to disaster risk reduction in Haiti (total earmarked disaster risk reduction and recovery funding for 2013 exceeds $68 million, UN OCHA, 2013), examination of factors with potential to impede training effectiveness is critical.

However, little is known about psychological factors contributing to the adoption or dismissal of preparedness measures in this population. In Western samples, factors such as locus of control and perceived risk of future disasters have been associated with preparedness efforts (Lindell & Perry, 2000; McClure, Walkey, & Allen, 1999; Spittal, McClure, Siegert, & Walkey, 2008), but such factors have not been adequately examined in other cultural contexts, developing countries, or in IDP settings. Even less is known about the role of mental health factors. Disasters are associated with severe mental health consequences (Norris, 2001; Norris et al. 2002). Posttraumatic stress and other forms of anxiety, depression, and somatic symptoms, including physical pain and sleep disturbances, are the most common forms of distress. In a meta-analysis of disaster survivors in 80 countries 25-49% of samples reported clinically significant psychological distress, with nearly one-quarter of these experiencing severe impairment (Norris, 2001; Norris et al. 2002).

For Haitian IDPs, who have almost universally endured disaster trauma, as well as other forms of trauma and the severe stress associated with survival in displaced contexts, the influence of mental health issues such as symptoms of PTSD and depression on preparedness efforts is particularly important. Although data documenting mental health of earthquake survivors in Haiti is scarce, existing research demonstrates significant emotional and somatic distress. A 2010 population-based study of 1324 adult earthquake survivors found that 29.7% of IDP camp residents met criteria for posttraumatic stress disorder, and another 28.8% met criteria for major depressive disorder, compared to 19.1% and 21.9% for PTSD and MDD respectively in community populations (Cerda et al., 2012). Risk factors for PTSD and MDD included pre-earthquake history of trauma, injury to self or family and damage or loss of home in the earthquake, and job loss and low social support after the earthquake. Prior studies suggest that many IDPs had significant trauma exposure and were at risk for depression and/or PTSD prior to the 2010 earthquake (Fawzi et al., 2009; Pierre et al., 2010). IDPs remaining in camps in late 2012 when Hurricane Sandy struck represent the most vulnerable of those displaced by the 2010 earthquake; they possess the fewest resources and have the most difficulty finding durable housing solutions (IASC, 2013) and are thus likely to be disproportionately affected by stress and trauma-related distress. Even among non-IDPs, earthquake-related exposure is associated with psychiatric morbidity. A 2011 post-earthquake household survey conducted in Haiti’s Central Plateau found that 41.7% of respondents met criteria for major depression and 6.1% endorsed current suicidal ideation. Although this study area did not directly experience damage from the 2010 earthquake, depression was associated with earthquake-related variables, including having a relative die and having others move into one’s house after the earthquake (Hagaman et al., 2013).

The relationship between distress and disaster preparedness engagement is thus far unclear. A number of studies find positive relationships among disaster exposure, distress, and preparedness, including among US earthquake and hurricane survivors and flood survivors in the Netherlands (e.g., Nguyen et al., 2006; Sattler, Kaiser, & Hittner, 2000; Siegel, Shoaf, Afifi, & Bourque, 2003; Zaalberg, Midden, Meijnders, & McCalley, 2009). Explanatory frameworks in these studies commonly draw on decision-theory perspectives which link disaster-related exposure and distress to perceived risk and/or vulnerability, which in turn promote increased preparedness (e.g., Lindell & Perry, 2012; 1992; see also conservation of resources stress model, Hobfoll, 1989).

However, distress symptoms also have potential to impede preparedness engagement through a variety of pathways, thus increasing vulnerability to disaster effects (Morrissey & Reser, 2003). For instance, feelings of hopelessness and powerlessness associated with depression may make preparedness feel overwhelming or pointless (e.g., Lin, Shaw, Ho, 2008). Additionally, for those traumatized by prior disaster exposure (such as the 2010 Haiti earthquake), avoidance of traumatic stimuli associated with PTSD (DSM-V, APA, 2013) may include efforts to avoid thinking about or preparing for future disasters. Perhaps surprisingly, these possibilities have not been thoroughly researched. The aim of this pilot study is to clarify the use of disaster preparedness measures in a Port-au-Prince vicinity camp for internally displaced peoples (IDPs) affected by Storm Sandy and to conduct a preliminary investigation of psychological factors related to the adoption of such measures. Specifically, this study collects data from focus groups and interviews conducted with a sample of IDP camp residents affected by Hurricane Sandy, with the objective of determining the extent to which mental health factors contribute to disaster preparedness engagement. This study will assess hypothesized relationships between disaster preparedness and trauma history (including disaster exposure), PTSD (with special attention to avoidance symptoms), and depression. Other variables identified in the literature as important for preparedness engagement were also assessed, namely trust in sources of preparedness information (see Samaddar, Misra, & Tatano, 2012), efficacy (as defined by perceived knowledge, money and time needed to prepare), and demographic variables, including gender, age, and income (e.g., Lindell & Hwang, 2008; Sattler et al., 2000; see Kellens, Terpstra, De Maeyer, 2013 for a review). This preliminary research is designed to inform future study and intervention development with potential to enhance engagement in disaster preparedness strategies in Haiti and other vulnerable populations in diverse disaster prone contexts.

Back to Top

General Methods

Staff and training
In March 2013, the PI trained a team of six Haitian research assistants to conduct focus groups and interviews and a project manager to provide oversight. Team members were veterans of a lay mental health worker project conducted by the PI from 2010-2012 (James, Noel, Favorite, & Jean, 2012; James & Noel, in press; James, Noel, & Jean Pierre, in press), and so had intensive training and prior experience in mental health-focused clinical and research practices, including interview and focus group research as part of needs assessments and evaluation studies. Research assistants were also students at Ensite Travay Sosyal ak Syans Sosyal (Institute of Social Work and Social Science) in Port-au-Prince, where the PI has a visiting faculty appointment. In exchange for their participation, they received field placement course credit and a financial stipend.

Study site
The Dye Done IDP camp in the Tabarre neighborhood of metropolitan Port-au-Prince was selected as the research site due to its relative lack of crime, its accessibility to research staff, and its location alongside a river, resulting in considerable flood damage from Hurricane Sandy. The PI and project manager secured permission from the Dye Done “camp committee” (the camp leadership contingency) to conduct research in the camp.

Focus Group Methods
The PI (with interpretation by research team members) conducted two Creole-language focus groups, the first consisting of all available camp committee members (n = 8) and the second of IDP camp residents (n = 14). Focus groups with these populations were conducted separately due to the status differential between groups, so as to protect against the possibility that responses by camp residents may be influenced by the presence of committee members (and vice-versa). The aim of both groups was 1) to identify appropriate preparedness and mitigation measures for the camp setting and determine the extent to which these have been implemented and 2) to discuss factors contributing to likelihood of implementation, including both resource-oriented and psychological components.

Committee member focus group recruitment. All committee members were invited to participate in a focus group. Each member was consented separately. After determining whether each member met recruitment criteria (age 18 or over; a resident of the Dye Done camp; living in the camp at the time of Hurricane Sandy), a research assistant read the informed consent document and members provided oral consent. No names were collected. All available committee members agreed to participate and provided consent. Participants were asked to report directly to a large tent for their focus group. Following participation in the group discussion (approximately 60 minutes), committee members were compensated with phone credit valuing $10. They also received a bottled drink and a snack.

Camp resident (non-committee member) focus group recruitment. To identify camp resident participants as randomly as possible, research assistants (working in pairs) began on opposite sides of the camp and approached the first adult visible outside of a tent or other dwelling, and then outside of every 5-10 dwellings to follow. Research assistants used a recruitment script to assess interest in participation and determine whether participants met recruitment criteria (age 18 or over; a resident of the Dye Done camp; living in the camp at the time of Hurricane Sandy). Those who expressed interest and met criteria were escorted to a location outside of earshot of other residents where research assistants read an informed consent document and participants were asked to provide oral consent. No names were collected. All residents approached expressed interest in participation and provided oral consent. Sixteen residents were invited to participate in a focus group and were provided with a time later the same day to report to a specified location. Fourteen residents arrived as planned. Following participation in the discussion (approximately 90 minutes), participants were compensated with phone credit valuing $5. They also received a bottled drink and a snack.

Analysis
Focus group content was audio-recorded, transcribed, and translated by the research team. Transcripts were reviewed by the PI and project manager to identify themes.

Focus Group Results
Strategies identified during focus group sessions included: listening to the radio or arranging to listen to someone else’s radio; creating an emergency kit with food, water, and medicine in case of evacuation; keeping important documents in a safe, waterproof place; arranging for disaster notifications on one’s cell phone, developing an evacuation and family meet-up plan, training children in these procedures, taking steps to stabilize one’s dwelling, and keeping important documents in safe places. Several behaviors were identified to be enacted once a hurricane warning is issued: tying down tents and digging canals. Preparedness strategies were incorporated into the structured interview schedule subsequently conducted with camp residents.

Back to Top

Interview Methods

Recruitment
Later the same week as the focus group, an additional sample of 74 camp residents were recruited for individual interviews using the methods described above for the camp resident focus groups. Again, all residents approached agreed to participate. Interviews were conducted in a quiet, private tent space provided by the camp committee. Participants were compensated with phone credit valuing $5, a bottled drink, and a snack.

Interview schedule
The interview schedule utilized the following measures. All were translated to Haitian Creole by the project manager through translation, back-translation, and revision for clarity.

Disaster preparedness: Participants were asked to state whether or not they had engaged in 10 disaster preparedness items (developed by investigators based on focus groups; informed by the Earthquake Readiness Scale by Spittal and colleagues (2008), see also Sattler et al., 2000). See Table 1 for items and descriptive statistics. A composite disaster preparedness variable was constructed using a sum of preparedness activities, M = 6.64, SD = 1.82. Two activities were excluded from the composite score; “Teach children what to do in the event of a disaster” was excluded because it was determined that participants who did not have children tended to state “no” rather than “not applicable” in response to this item, and “Sign up to receive disaster warnings or information by text message” was excluded because it was determined that some cell phone companies automatically send warning messages so responses were determined by cell phone possession rather than preparedness engagement.

Preparedness efficacy. Participants were asked three questions taken from the Vested Interest Scale (Miller, Adame, & Moore, 2013) to measure perceived efficacy to prepare for disasters. Specifically, participants were asked to rate responses to the following questions on 7-point Likert scales: 1) “How able are you to take the time to prepare for a disaster?” (M = 3.23, SD = 2.11, range: 1-7); 2) “Can you afford to buy the items needed to prepare for disaster?” (M = 2.68, SD = 2.24, range: 1-7); and 3) “How much knowledge do you have about how to prepare for a disaster?” (M = 4.78, SD = 1.85, range: 1-7). Items formed a reliable scale (a = .789) so were averaged to create a single efficacy item, M = 10.69, SD = 5.22, range: 3-21.

Trauma exposure. Using an adapted Life Events Checklist (Blake, Weathers, Nagy, Kaloupek, Charney, & Keane, 1995), participants were asked to state whether or not 12 disaster exposure items and 8 non-disaster trauma exposure items “happened to me”. See Table 2 for items and descriptive statistics. A trauma exposure composite variable was created by summing items (two items, earthquake and hurricane, were excluded because nearly every respondent endorsed having these experiences; one item, tsunami, was excluded because almost no respondents endorsed having this experience) (M = 7.53, SD = 4.33, range: 2-16).

Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms. The PTSD checklist portion of the Harvard Trauma Questionnaire (HTQ), created by the Harvard Program for Refugee Trauma, aims to assess the mental health of survey and interview respondents who have experienced displacement, disaster, and war (Mollica et al., 1992). It is designed specifically for cross-cultural use and has been implemented in a wide variety of settings (Mollica et al., 2004), including Haiti both before and after the earthquake (Kolbe et al., unpublished). The following assessments used only the 4th section of the HTQ, which consists of 16 self-report questions modeled after PTSD diagnostic criteria of the DSM-IV. Participants rate how much symptoms have bothered them in the last week on a four point scale ranging from “not at all” to “extremely”. Scores are averaged for the 16 items, with those with scores of 2.5 or above meeting cut-off criteria for PTSD 1 (Mollica et al., 1992). The mean HTQ score was 2.65 (SD = .77; range: 1.19-3.94), with 66.2% of respondents scoring 2.5 or above.

In line with evidence supporting a four-factor model of PTSD in which avoidance and numbing symptoms are distinct constructs (e.g., Asmundson, Stapleton, & Taylor, 2004), and to test hypotheses regarding the influence of avoidance symptoms of PTSD, items were also summed to create four symptom cluster scores: re-experiencing/intrusion (M = 2.77, SD = .92), avoidance (M = 2.5, SD =1.10), numbing (M = 2.47, SD = .73), and hyperarousal (M = 2.79, SD = .89). See Table 3 for items, clustering, and descriptive statistics by item.

Depression symptoms. Assessment of depression utilized the 16-item Hopkins Symptom Checklist (Derogatis, Lipman, Rickels, Uhlenhuth, & Covi, 2007). Participants rated how much symptoms have bothered them in the last week on a four point scale ranging from “not at all” to “extremely”. A composite depression score was constructed by summing item scores, M = 36.77, SD = 11.95, range: 15-57. See Table 4 for items and descriptive statistics.

Credibility of disaster information. Participants were asked to rate how likely they would be to act on disaster preparedness information from eight sources (e.g., the Haitian government; family and friends) using a five-point Likert Scale. See Table 5 for items and descriptive statistics. Items formed a reliable scale (a = .870) so were averaged to create a single source credibility item, M = 3.11, SD = 1.19.

Demographic information. Gender, age, number of children, marital status, relative financial security, employment and student status, and religion, were also assessed. Seventy-four (41 female, 34 male) participants were interviewed, with mean age of 32.4 years (SD = 9.93, range: 18-63). Participants reported a mean of 2.3 children (range: 0-8) and 20% reported that they were married. Sixteen percent of participants reported being currently employed and 20.3% reported current student status. To assess relative financial well-being, participants responded to “How well off is your family compared to others in the community?” on a five point scale (1 = “Much less than”; 5 = “Much more than”) (M = 3.20, SD = .97, range: 1-5).

The survey also included additional variables designed to assess cultural and religious factors, coping, and perceived risk, for use in future analyses to test additional hypotheses not associated with the current paper.

Analysis.
Data were entered into an excel file by the interviewers and sent to the PI for analysis. Multiple regression analyses were used to examine the relationship between mental health predictor variables and disaster preparedness variables, controlling for demographic factors as appropriate.

Interview Results
Multiple regression analyses were used to test hypothesized mental health predictors of disaster preparedness engagement: trauma exposure, PTSD symptoms (particularly avoidance symptoms) and depression symptoms. To test the hypothesis that avoidance symptoms of PTSD are particularly relevant in predicting preparedness engagement, rather than using a single HTQ sum score, four variables representing each of the PTSD symptom clusters (re-experiencing, avoidance, numbing and hyperarousal) were included in the model. Disaster preparedness efficacy, source credibility, and relative financial security 2 were also included in the model.

The results of the regression indicated that the predictors explained 67.3% of the variance (R2 = .673, F(9,61) = 13.96, p < .001). Trauma exposure significantly predicted disaster preparedness, such that those with more exposure were less likely to engage in preparedness behaviors. The avoidance symptom cluster of PTSD surfaced as a significant predictor of preparedness, such that avoidance symptoms were associated with less preparedness behavior. However, there were no significant effects of other PTSD symptom clusters or of the depression scale. Preparedness efficacy and relative financial well-being were associated with increased preparedness, while higher perceptions of source credibility were associated with less preparedness engagement. See Table 6 for betas and significance levels.

Back to Top

Discussion

Analyses revealed that engagement in disaster preparedness was predicted by trauma exposure and avoidance symptoms of PTSD. There was a negative relationship between trauma exposure and disaster preparedness, such that increased trauma (including disaster exposure) was associated with decreased preparedness. Results are congruent with findings by Lin, Shaw, and Ho (2008) showing that Taiwanese flood and landslide victims were less likely than the general public to engage in activities such as relocating, purchasing insurance, or otherwise inconveniencing oneself or taking on expense to mitigate disaster. However, in some ways, this is a counter-intuitive finding in that exposure to prior trauma, including prior disasters, might be predicted to evoke increase preparedness due to greater and more salient knowledge regarding the risk and consequences of negative disaster impact. Indeed, in their review, Kellens and colleagues (2013) report that most studies find a positive relationship between hazards exposure and disaster mitigation. They suggest that effects of disaster exposure may be mediated by other (often psychological) variables.

While other researchers have focused on psychological factors such as perceived risk and vulnerability (Kellens et al., 2013), the current study focused on the relevance of mental health symptoms, including those potentially stemming from prior disaster-related and non-disaster trauma and stress. In these data, avoidance symptoms of PTSD (“Avoiding activities that remind you of the traumatic or hurtful event predicted decreased preparedness” and “Avoiding thoughts or feelings associated with the experience”) was the only PTSD symptom cluster significantly predictive of preparedness. Presumably, avoidance symptoms related to prior disaster trauma (such as death of close others or being trapped in the 2010 earthquake) may discourage engagement with disaster-related content of all sorts – including preparedness efforts for future disasters. Interestingly, despite an intuitive connection between hopeless and helplessness associated with depression and decreased preparedness efforts, the depression scale was not a significant predictor of preparedness in this model. Further work is needed to assess the extent to which these early indications reflect consistent patterns; if so, results imply that attention may be better focused on decreasing avoidance symptoms than on resolving feelings of hopelessness in this population.

Trust of sources of disaster information was a significant predictor of preparedness engagement. Interestingly, while prior studies have found a positive relationship between trust and disaster mitigation (e.g., Samaddar, Misra, & Tatano, 2012), the opposite relationship surfaced in these data, such that increased confidence in sources of information was associated with decreased preparedness. While this initially appears contradictory, it may be explained by several factors, including the exceedingly high levels of corruption and low levels of trust in Haitian society (e.g., Pierre et al, 2010), and the fact that disaster preparedness activities assessed in this study were individual and family level, rather than organizational-level efforts (such as coordinated evacuation efforts). Respondents may have perceived this kind of preparedness ass more important when authorities cannot be trusted to help or provide accurate information in times of crisis.

In line with the literature (see Kellens et al., 2013), disaster preparedness efficacy, as defined by perceived possession of necessary finances, time, and knowledge, was also a significant predictor of preparedness, as was perceived financial security overall (relative to other community members). Results suggest that both mental health and other perceptions of preparedness ability are necessary contributors to self-reported engagement.

Limitations and implications
The current analyses are preliminary. Future analyses will build on the current regression model by incorporating cultural and religious variables potentially contributing to psychological processes among Haitian participants (see Pierre et al., 2010). Moreover, mediation and moderation analyses will be used to assess the extent to which the relationship between trauma and disaster exposure and disaster preparedness is explained by specific symptoms of PTSD and depression. A more rigorous analysis of the qualitative focus group data will allow for further assessment of pathways by which disaster preparedness is impeded as perceived by IDP camp residents. Generalizability of findings associated with this study is limited by the relatively small sample size and restriction to a single IDP camp. Additionally, the cross-sectional nature of this study impedes determination of causality (e.g., between avoidance and decreased preparedness). A longitudinal model would allow for assessment of the predictive power of avoidance over time. Finally, disaster preparedness is assessed by self report; a behavioral measure would allow for a more accurate assessment of preparedness engagement.

Despite limitations, the current study has significant implications for intervention. Haitian IDPs continue to be at significant risk of disaster exposure; as of April 2013, 86 camps, harboring nearly 40,000 families, are considered at high risk of further hurricane impact in metropolitan Port-au-Prince alone, according to the Inter-Agency Standing Committee in Haiti (Adams & Mellicker, 2013). In particular, the current results imply the importance of involving mental health-focused intervention in disaster risk reduction (DRR) efforts. By acknowledging psychological factors such as avoidance that may impede preparedness and teaching relevant coping skills, DRR trainers may increase the likelihood that trainees will be able to fully absorb training preparedness and mitigation content and translate knowledge to action.

For chronically disaster-exposed populations such as Haitian IDPs, a joint mental health and DRR initiative makes intuitive sense from both a prevention and treatment perspective. Just as mental health intervention may improve preparedness, preparedness training is in turn likely to benefit mental health. Disasters adversely impact mental health so to the extent that disaster impact can be mitigated, disaster-related distress can be prevented or decreased (Norris, 2001; Norris et al. 2002). In the shorter term, DRR training may improve participant well-being by increasing perceived safety and efficacy to keep one’s family and property safe in the aftermath of a feared future disaster, by facilitating building of social ties, and by providing an empowering opportunity to disseminate useful information to others. Indeed, there is evidence that disaster preparedness volunteerism is associated with posttraumatic growth among earthquake survivors (Karanci & Acarturk, 2005).

In sum, by beginning to clarify the pathways by which distress may affect disaster preparedness, the current study makes an empirical contribution to the development of a theoretical framework that can inform intervention models tailored to chronically disaster exposed populations in Haiti and elsewhere.

Back to Top

References

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.

Asmundson, G. J., Stapleton, J. A., & Taylor, S. (2004). Are avoidance and numbing distinct PTSD symptom clusters?. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 17(6), 467-475.

Adams, H. & Mellicker, B. (2013). Fact sheet-UCLBP & shelter and ccm cluster Haiti-April 2013. Inter-Agency Standing Committee. Retrieved from: http://www.eshelter-cccmhaiti.info/jl/pdf/2013/Cluster-CCCMShelter_Avril-2013.pdf

Blake, D. D., Weathers, F. W., Nagy, L. M., Kaloupek, D. G., Gusman, F. D., Charney, D. S., & Keane, T. M. (1995). The development of a clinician administered PTSD scale. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 8(1), 75-90.

Cerda, M., Paczkowski, M., Galea, S., Nemethy, K., Péan, C., & Desvarieux, M. (2012). Psychopathology in the aftermath of the Haiti earthquake: a population-based study of posttraumatic stress disorder and major depression. Depression and Anxiety, 30(5), 413-424.

Derogatis, L. R., Lipman, R. S., Rickels, K., Uhlenhuth, E. H., & Covi, L. (2007). The Hopkins Symptom Checklist: A self-report symptom inventory. Behavioral Science, 19(1), 1-15.

Fawzi, M. C., Betancourt, T. S., Marcelin, L., Klopner, M., Munir, K., Muriel, A. C., …& Mukherjee, J. S. (2009). Depression and post-traumatic stress disorder among Haitian immigrant students: Implications for access to mental health services and educational programming. BMC Public Health, 9(1), 482.

Hagaman, A. K., Wagenaar, B. H., McLean, K. E., Kaiser, B. N., Winskell, K., & Kohrt, B. A. (2013). Suicide in rural Haiti: Clinical and community perceptions of prevalence, etiology, and prevention. Social Science & Medicine.

Hobfoll, S. E. (1989). Conservation of resources: A new attempt at conceptualizing stress. American psychologist, 44(3), 513.

Inter-Agency Standing Committee (IASC). (2013). Displacement Tracking Matrix. Available at: http://www.eshelterccmhaiti.info/jl/index.php?option=com_content&id=11&Url.

Internal Displacement Monitoring Centre (IDMC). (16 November 2012). Haiti: Tropical Storm Sandy displaces 35,000 people, while over 31,000 earthquake IDPs are hit again. Available at: http://www.unhcr.org/refworld/docid/50acb3ec2.html [accessed 4 December 2012]

International Organization for Migration (IOM). (2012). Haiti: From Emergency to Sustainable Recovery: IOM Haiti Two-year Report 2010-2011. Port-au-Prince, Haiti: International Organization for Migration Haiti. Available at: http://www.iom.int/jahia/webdav/shared/shared/mainsite/activities/countries/docs/haiti/IOM-Haiti-Two-Year-Report-2010-2011-From-Emergency-to-Sustainable-Recovery.pdf [accessed 4 December 2012]

International Organization for Migration (IOM). (2013). IOM-Haiti Strategic Plan 2013- 2014. Retrieved from: http://www.iom.int/files/live/sites/iom/files/pbn/docs/Strategy-IOM-Haiti-Updated-Jan-2013.pdf

James, L. E. & Noel, J. R. (in press). Lay mental health in the aftermath of disaster: Preliminary evaluation of an intervention for Haiti earthquake survivors. International Journal of Emergency Mental Health.

James, L. E., Noel, J. R, Jean Pierre, M. R. (in press). A mixed-methods assessment of the experiences of lay mental health workers in post-earthquake Haiti. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry.

James, L. E., Noel, J. R., Favorite, T. K., & Jean, J. S. (2012). Challenges of Postdisaster Intervention in Cultural Context. International Perspectives in Psychology: Research, Practice, and Consultation, 1(2), 110-126.

Karanci, N. A. (2005). Post-traumatic growth among Marmara earthquake survivors involved in disaster preparedness as volunteers. Traumatology, 11(4), 307-323.

Kellens, W., Terpstra, T., & De Maeyer, P. (2013). Perception and communication of flood risks: A systematic review of empirical research. Risk Analysis, 33(1), 24-49.

Lin, S., Shaw, D., & Ho, M. C. (2008). Why are flood and landslide victims less willing to take mitigation measures than the public?. Natural Hazards, 44(2), 305-314.

Lindell, M. K., & Hwang, S. N. (2008). Households' perceived personal risk and responses in a multihazard environment. Risk Analysis, 28(2), 539-556.

Lindell, M. K., & Perry, R. W. (1992). Behavioral foundations of community emergency planning. Hemisphere Publishing Corp.

Lindell, M. K., & Perry, R. W. (2000). Household Adjustment to Earthquake Hazard: A Review of Research. Environment and Behavior, 32(4), 461-501.

Lindell, M. K., & Perry, R. W. (2012). The protective action decision model: theoretical modifications and additional evidence. Risk Analysis, 32(4), 616-632.

McClure, J., Walkey, F., & Allen, M. (1999). When earthquake damage is seen as preventable: Attributions, locus of control and attitudes to risk. Applied Psychology, 48(2), 239-256.

McKay, J. (2012). The preparedness message isn’t reaching the public. Retrieved from the Emergency Management website: http://www.emergencymgmt.com/disaster/Preparedness-Message-Isnt-Reaching-Public.html

Miller, C. H., Adame, B. J., & Moore, S. D. (2013). Vested Interest theory and disaster preparedness. Disasters, 37(1), 1-27.

Mollica, R. F. (2004). Measuring Trauma, Measuring Torture: Instructions and Guidance on the Utilization of the Harvard Program in Refugee Trauma's Versions of the Hopkins Symptom Checklist-25 (HSCL-25) & the Harvard Trauma Questionnaire (HTQ). Harvard Program in Refugee Trauma.

Mollica, R. F., Caspi-Yavin, Y., Bollini, P., & Truong, T. (1992). The Harvard Trauma Questionnaire: validating a cross-cultural instrument for measuring torture, trauma, and posttraumatic stress disorder in Indochinese refugees. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 180(2), 111-116.

Morrissey, S. A., & Reser, J. P. (2003). Evaluating the effectiveness of psychological preparedness advice in community cyclone preparedness materials. Australian Journal of Emergency Management, 18(2), 46-61.

Norris, F. H., Byrne, C. M., Diaz, E., & Kaniasty, K. (2001). 50,000 disaster victims speak: An empirical review of the empirical literature, 1981-2001. Department of Health and Human Services. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration.

Norris, F. H., Friedman, M. J., Watson, P. J., Byrne, C. M., Diaz, E., & Kaniasty, K. (2002). 60,000 disaster victims speak: Part I. An empirical review of the empirical literature, 1981–2001. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 65(3), 207-239.

Nguyen, L. H., Shen, H., Ershoff, D., Afifi, A. A., & Bourque, L. B. (2006). Exploring the causal relationship between exposure to the 1994 Northridge earthquake and pre-and post-earthquake preparedness activities. Earthquake Spectra, 22(3), 569-587.

Pierre, A., Minn, P., Sterlin, C., Annoual, P. C., Jaimes, A., Raphaël, F., ... & Kirmayer, L. J. (2010). Culture and mental health in Haiti: A literature review. Santé Mentale en Haïti, 1, 13-42.

Reale, A. (2010). Acts of God (s): The role of religion in Disaster Risk Reduction. Humanitarian Exchange Magazine, 48.

Samaddar, S., Misra, B. A., & Tatano, H. (2012, October). Flood risk awareness and preparedness: The role of trust in information sources. In Systems, Man, and Cybernetics (SMC), 2012 IEEE International Conference on (pp. 3099-3104). IEEE.

Sattler, D. N., Kaiser, C. F., & Hittner, J. B. (2000). Disaster Preparedness: Relationships Among Prior Experience, Personal Characteristics, and Distress. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30(7), 1396-1420.

Siegel, J. M., Shoaf, K. I., Afifi, A. A., & Bourque, L. B. (2003). Surviving Two Disasters Does Reaction to the First Predict Response to the Second? Environment and Behavior, 35(5), 637-654.

Spittal, M. J., McClure, J., Siegert, R. J., & Walkey, F. H. (2008). Predictors of Two Types of Earthquake Preparation Survival Activities and Mitigation Activities. Environment and Behavior, 40(6), 798-817.

United Nations Office for the Coordination of Humanitarian Affairs (OCHA). (2012). Humanitarian Action Plan for Haiti 2013. Retrieved from www.unocha.org/cap/appeals/humanitarian-action-plan-haiti-2013

United Nations Office for the Coordination of Humanitarian Affairs (OCHA). Financial Tracking Service (FTS). (2013). Haiti 2013: Total Humanitarian Funding. Retrieved from http://fts.unocha.org/pageloader.aspx?page=emerg-emergencyDetails&appealID=1008

USAID/OFDA. (2011, March 4). Haiti—Earthquake: Fact sheet #11. Retrieved from http://www.usaidgov/our_work/humanitarian_assistance/disaster_assistance/countries/haiti/template/fs_sr/fy2011/haiti_eq_fs11_03-04-2011.pdf

United States Agency for International Development (USAID). (2012). Most Recent Disaster Declaration: Haiti Floods. 31 October 2012. Available at: http://transition.usaid.gov/our_work/humanitarian_assistance/disaster_assistance/countries/haiti/template/index.html [accessed 4 December 2012]

Yeager, V. A., Menachemi, N., McCormick, L. C., & Ginter, P. M. (2010). The nature of the public health emergency preparedness literature 2000-2008: A quantitative analysis. Journal of Public Health Management and Practice, 16(5), 441-449.

Zaalberg, R., Midden, C., Meijnders, A., & McCalley, T. (2009). Prevention, adaptation, and threat denial: Flooding experiences in the Netherlands. Risk Analysis, 29(12), 1759-1778.

Footnotes

1. These criteria have not been validated with this population and should be interpreted as a marker of severity of distress rather than as diagnostic of PTSD.

2. To determine which demographic variables to include in regression analyses, those variables found to be significantly related to preparedness in the literature (gender, age, relative financial security; see Kellens et al., 2013) were included in a correlation matrix with disaster preparedness. Only relative financial security was significantly correlated (r = .246, p = .035) with preparedness so was included in the current model.

Back to Top

Tables

Table 1. Descriptive Statistics of Disaster Preparedness Variables
Variable Endorsement Frequency Endorsement Valid Percent
1. Discuss or write down an evacuation plan (where to go if you need to leave the camp – e.g. friend or family’s house) 56 75.7
2. Discuss or write down a plan for reconnecting with family if separated 53 71.6
3. Teach (talk to) children what to do in the event of a disaster 47 63.5
4. Sign up to receive disaster warnings or information by text message 68 91.9
5. Listen to the radio or watch TV to check for adverse weather events, or disaster warnings 71 95.9
6. Put important documents and other possessions in a safe place (e.g. in a plastic bag) 71 95.9
7. Learn (study, hear about) basic first aid (e.g., how to stop bleeding, what to do if someone breaks a leg, CPR) 47 63.5
8. Learn (study, hear about) about how to be healthy and hygienic after a disaster (e.g. how to access clean water ) 67 90.5
9. Secure dwelling from wind and rain or make stronger in some other way (to withstand an earthquake for example) 63 85.1
10. Make a disaster supply kit (e.g. with extra water, food, first aid supplies, candles, matches) 63 85.4

Return to text ↑

Table 2. Descriptive Statistics of Disaster Exposure Variables
Variable Endorsement Frequency Endorsement Valid Percent
Disaster Exposure
1. Earthquake 74 100
2. Hurricane 73 98.6
3. Flood 48 64.9
4. Tsunami 1 1.3
5. Cholera or other disaster epidemic 55 74.3
6. Fire/Explosion 44 59.5
7. House destroyed or badly damaged due to disaster 64 86.5
8. Injury in a Disaster 27 36
9. Trapped under rubble in a disaster 33 44.6
10. Close friends or family injured in a disaster 59 79.7
11. Close friends or family killed by a disaster 50 67.6
Non-disaster trauma exposure
12. Transportation accident 30 40.5
13. Other serious accident 22 29.7
14. Physical Assault 27 36.5
15. Unwanted or uncomfortable sexual experience including sexual assault 11 14.9
16. Combat or exposure to political violence or civil conflict 8 10.7
17. Sudden violent death of someone close to you 27 36
18. Sudden violent death of someone else (e.g. a stranger in a traffic accident) 22 29.7
19. Serious injury, harm, or death you caused to someone else 30 40.5

Return to text ↑

Table 3. Descriptive Statistics of PTSD symptom items (Harvard Trauma Questionnaire)

Variable

Mean

SD

1. Recurrent thoughts or memories of hurtful or terrifying events (re-experiencing)

2.49

1.26

2. Feeling as though the hurtful or terrifying event is happening again (re-experiencing)

2.82

1.11

3. Recurrent nightmares (re-experiencing)

2.84

1.30

4. Feeling detached or withdrawn from people (numbing)

2.38

1.16

5. Unable to feel emotions (numbing)

2.54

1.19

6. Feeling jumpy, easily startled (hyperarousal)

2.97

1.26

7. Difficulty concentrating (hyperarousal)

3.03

1.12

8. Trouble sleeping (hyperarousal)

2.73

1.19

9. Feeling on guard (hyperarousal)

2.82

1.02

10. Feeling irritable or having angry outbursts (hyperarousal)

2.44

1.23

11. Avoiding activities that remind you of the traumatic or hurtful event (avoidance)

2.66

1.20

12. Inability to remember parts of the most traumatic or hurtful event (numbing)

2.16

1.30

13. Less interest in daily activities (numbing)

2.88

1.02

14. Feeling as if you don’t have a future (numbing)

2.38

1.25

15. Avoiding thoughts or feelings associated with the experience (avoidance)

2.38

1.19

16. Sudden emotional or physical reaction when reminder of the event (re-experiencing)

2.95

1.25

Note: Items utilized a 4-point Likert scale (1 = “not at all”; 4 = “extremely”). Return to text ↑



Table 4. Descriptive Statistics of Depression Symptom Items (Hopkins Symptom Checklist)

Variable

Mean

SD

1. Feeling low in energy, slowed down

2.89

1.14

2. Blaming yourself for things

2.54

1.33

3. Crying easily

2.28

1.28

4. Loss of sexual interest or pleasure

2.42

1.27

5. Poor appetite

2.42

1.17

6. Difficulty falling asleep, staying asleep

2.68

1.12

7. Feeling hopeless about the future

2.27

1.86

8. Feeling blue (sad)

3.03

1.02

9. Feeling lonely

2.92

1.10

10. Thoughts of ending your life

1.54

0.92

11. Feelings of being trapped or caught (no way to escape from your problems)

2.19

1.21

12. Worry too much about things

2.47

1.13

13. Feeling no interest in things in life in general

2.39

1.17

14. Feeling everything is an effort

2.42

1.06

15. Feeling of worthlessness

2.31

1.23

16. Feeling hopeless about the future

2.95

1.25

Note: Items utilized a 4-point Likert scale (1 = “not at all”; 4 = “extremely”). Return to text ↑



Table 5. Descriptive Statistics of Credibility of Disaster Information Items

How likely are you to act on disaster preparedness information from these sources?

Mean

SD

1. Haitian government

2.86

1.80

2. MINUSTAH (UN peacekeeping mission)

2.49

1.71

3. Camp committee

2.34

1.71

4. Other camp residents/local Haitian people

2.69

1.61

5. Foreigners working with an international organization

3.78

1.68

6. Haitian people working with an international organization

2.85

1.82

7. Your religious leader

3.82

1.40

8. Your family and friends

4.03

1.40

Note: Items utilized a 5-point Likert scale (1 = “definitely not act”; 5 = “definitely act”). Return to text ↑



Table 6. Predictors of Disaster Preparedness

Unstandardized Coefficients

Standardized Coefficients

B

Std. Error

Beta

t

Sig.

Constant

6.885

1.064

6.470

.000

PTSD Reexperiencing

.063

.071

.125

.887

.379

PTSD Hyperarousal

-.004

.059

-.009

-.065

.948

PTSD Avoidance

-.268

.119

-.312

-2.252

.028

PTSD Numbing

.085

.069

.167

1.232

.223

Depression

.010

.023

.063

.428

.670

Trauma exposure

-.157

.048

-.399

-3.386

.002

Self-efficacy scale

.271

.098

.258

2.774

.007

Source credibility

-.711

.162

-.466

-4.390

.000

Relative well-off

.478

.179

.248

2.678

.010

Return to text ↑

Back to Top





Natural Hazards Center
Natural Hazards Center
483 UCB
Boulder, CO 80309-0483
Contact Us: hazctr@colorado.edu | (303) 492-6818